首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
The intrinsic neuronal code that carries visual information and the perceptual mechanism for decoding that information are not known. However, multivariate statistics and information theory show that neurons in four visual areas simultaneously carry multiple, stimulus-related messages by utilizing multiplexed temporal codes. The complexity of these temporal messages increases progressively across the visual system, yet the temporal codes overlap in time. Thus, visual perception may depend on the concurrent processing of multiplexed temporal messages from all visual areas.  相似文献   

2.
The deficits in texture, motion, and depth perception incurred in monkeys at isoluminance were compared with the responses of neurons of the color-opponent and broad-band systems in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Texture perception, assumed to be carried by the color-opponent system, and motion and depth perception, ascribed to the broad-band pathway, were all found to be compromised but not abolished at isoluminance. Correspondingly, both the color-opponent and the broad-band systems were affected at isoluminance, but the activity of the neurons in neither system was abolished. These results suggest that impairment of visual capacities at isoluminance cannot be uniquely attributed to either of these systems and that isoluminant stimuli are inappropriate for the psychophysical isolation of these pathways.  相似文献   

3.
Incoming sensory information is often ambiguous, and the brain has to make decisions during perception. "Predictive coding" proposes that the brain resolves perceptual ambiguity by anticipating the forthcoming sensory environment, generating a template against which to match observed sensory evidence. We observed a neural representation of predicted perception in the medial frontal cortex, while human subjects decided whether visual objects were faces or not. Moreover, perceptual decisions about faces were associated with an increase in top-down connectivity from the frontal cortex to face-sensitive visual areas, consistent with the matching of predicted and observed evidence for the presence of faces.  相似文献   

4.
The cerebral cortex of the human brain is a sheet of about 10 billion neurons divided into discrete subdivisions or areas that process particular aspects of sensation, movement, and cognition. Recent evidence has begun to transform our understanding of how cortical areas form, make specific connections with other brain regions, develop unique processing networks, and adapt to changes in inputs.  相似文献   

5.
Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception   总被引:10,自引:0,他引:10  
Neuronal activity in the superior temporal sulcus of monkeys, a cortical region that plays an important role in analyzing visual motion, was related to the subjective perception of movement during a visual task. Single neurons were recorded while monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminated the direction of motion of stimuli that could be seen moving in either of two directions during binocular rivalry. The activity of many neurons was dictated by the retinal stimulus. Other neurons, however, reflected the monkeys' reported perception of motion direction, indicating that these neurons in the superior temporal sulcus may mediate the perceptual experience of a moving object.  相似文献   

6.
Vision in most vertebrates is an active process that requires the brain to combine retinal signals with information about eye movement. Eye movement information may feed forward from the motor control areas of the brain or feed back from the extrinsic eye muscles. Feedback signals elicited by passive eye movement selectively gate retinal outflow at the first relay, the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. The gating predominantly facilitates retinogeniculate transmission immediately after eye movement and inhibits transmission when a new steady-state eye position is achieved. These two gating effects are distributed in a complementary fashion across the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus such that the spatiotemporal activity profile could contribute to object detection and localization.  相似文献   

7.
储可可 《安徽农业科学》2010,38(6):3251-3253
在现代景观的体验过程中,色彩是由视觉而感知的,它既依附于具象的实体,也依赖于光这一抽象的实体而存在。该研究通过分析色彩的视觉属性、心理知觉,对现代景观设计中的色彩进行解析,从色彩在场所文化中的应用、色彩技法在景观设计中的体现几方面来阐述色彩在景观设计中的应用类型和应用方法。  相似文献   

8.
In order to achieve high competitive quality of bamboo products, it appears that bamboo strips with naturally different tonalities should be elaborately sorted into different classes according to their global color texture appearance. Inspired by the coarse-to-fine visual perception process of human vision system, this paper proposes a new surface grading approach by integrating the color and texture of bamboo strips based on Gaussian multi-scale space. The multi-scale representations of color texture for the original image of bamboo strips could be obtained and used to construct the multivariate image, each channel of which represents a perceptual observation from different scales. The multivariate image analysis (MIA) techniques are used to extract multi-scale features from the resulting multivariate image data. The characteristic images corresponding to typical classes are selected to build the model of the reference eigenspace. The novel testing images and the training images are all projected onto this reference eigenspace to obtain their representative feature clusters. And the Bhattacharyya distance is used to estimate the similarity of the representative feature clusters between the testing images and the training images in the eigenspace. Then a k-NN classifier is adopted to classify the testing images into the given classes of training images. Comparative experiments have been carried out on a set of actual bamboo strip images and the experimental results verify the effective discrimination of multi-scale color texture eigenspace features and good classification accuracy of the proposed surface grading method.  相似文献   

9.
Experimentally induced visual projections into auditory thalamus and cortex   总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13  
Retinal cells have been induced to project into the medial geniculate nucleus, the principal auditory thalamic nucleus, in newborn ferrets by reduction of targets of retinal axons in one hemisphere and creation of alternative terminal space for these fibers in the auditory thalamus. Many cells in the medial geniculate nucleus are then visually driven, have large receptive fields, and receive input from retinal ganglion cells with small somata and slow conduction velocities. Visual cells with long conduction latencies and large contralateral receptive fields can also be recorded in primary auditory cortex. Some visual cells in auditory cortex are direction selective or have oriented receptive fields that resemble those of complex cells in primary visual cortex. Thus, functional visual projections can be routed into nonvisual structures in higher mammals, suggesting that the modality of a sensory thalamic nucleus or cortical area may be specified by its inputs during development.  相似文献   

10.
The principal eyes of jumping spiders have a unique retina with four tiered photoreceptor layers, on each of which light of different wavelengths is focused by a lens with appreciable chromatic aberration. We found that all photoreceptors in both the deepest and second-deepest layers contain a green-sensitive visual pigment, although green light is only focused on the deepest layer. This mismatch indicates that the second-deepest layer always receives defocused images, which contain depth information of the scene in optical theory. Behavioral experiments revealed that depth perception in the spider was affected by the wavelength of the illuminating light, which affects the amount of defocus in the images resulting from chromatic aberration. Therefore, we propose a depth perception mechanism based on how much the retinal image is defocused.  相似文献   

11.
The responses of single units in the monkey lateral geniculate nucleus to different portions of figures which differed from their backgrounds in color and brightness were examined. Border enhancement was found in the response to luminance figures but not in the response to color figures. In addition, cells showed border enhancement only in the case of a figure which produced an increment (as opposed to a decrement) in their firing rates. In situations in which very striking brightness contrast is seen perceptually, the cells do not show the corresponding changes in firing rate across the whole pattern. The lateral inhibitory mechanisms found in the retina and geniculate can thus account for luminance border enhancement, but not entirely for simultaneous brightness or color contrast, for which other cortical processes of some sort must be responsible.  相似文献   

12.
研究火力楠心材与边材的颜色和物理力学性质,为该树种木材的合理开发和利用提供参考。采用CIE Lab颜色系统表征木材的颜色参数(明度指数L*、红绿轴色品指数a*、黄蓝轴色品指数b*);按照国家标准测量木材的物理性质(密度、干缩性、湿胀性)和力学性质(顺纹抗压强度、横纹抗压强度、抗弯强度、抗弯弹性模量、顺纹抗剪强度、硬度、冲击韧性、顺纹抗拉强度、抗劈力、握钉力)。结果表明,火力楠心材的L*a*b*均小于边材,表明心材的颜色偏向黑色的程度更大,而偏向红色和黄色的程度更小,整体颜色更加暗深;火力楠心材与边材的总色差属人视觉感觉差异“可察觉”。火力楠心材的密度小于边材,二者密度均属中等;心材的差异干缩大于边材,二者气干干缩的不均匀性均属小;心材的体积干缩系数小于边材,二者干缩性均属很小。火力楠边材的吸水增重率大于心材,表明边材的渗透性优于心材。火力楠边材的硬度、冲击韧性、抗劈力和握钉力大于心材,而其余力学指标小于心材。火力楠心材和边材的顺纹抗压强度、抗弯强度、顺纹抗剪强度、端面硬度和冲击韧性,均属木材各项力学指标品质分级的中等及以上水平。火力楠心材的综合强度和综合品质系数均大于边材,二者均分别属高强度和强重比很高。  相似文献   

13.
The development of orderly connections in the mammalian visual system depends on action potentials in the optic nerve fibers, even before the retina receives visual input. In particular, it has been suggested that correlated firing of retinal ganglion cells in the same eye directs the segregation of their synaptic terminals into eye-specific layers within the lateral geniculate nucleus. Such correlations in electrical activity were found by simultaneous recording of the extracellular action potentials of up to 100 ganglion cells in the isolated retina of the newborn ferret and the fetal cat. These neurons fired spikes in nearly synchronous bursts lasting a few seconds and separated by 1 to 2 minutes of silence. Individual bursts consisted of a wave of excitation, several hundred micrometers wide, sweeping across the retina at about 100 micrometers per second. These concerted firing patterns have the appropriate spatial and temporal properties to guide the refinement of connections between the retina and the lateral geniculate nucleus.  相似文献   

14.
Objects displaced intermittently across the visual field will nonetheless give an illusion of continuous motion [called apparent motion (AM)] under many common conditions. It is believed that form perception is of minor importance in determining AM, and that AM is mediated by motion-sensitive areas in the "where" pathway of the cortex. However, form and motion typically interact in specific ways when natural objects move through the environment. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to measure cortical activation to long-range AM, compared to short-range AM and flicker, while we varied stability of structural differences between forms. Long-range AM activated the anterior-temporal lobe in the visual ventral pathway, and the response varied according to the form stability. The results suggest that long-range AM is associated with neural systems for form perception.  相似文献   

15.
The anterior part of the macaque inferior temporal cortex, area TE, occupies a large portion of the temporal lobe and is critical for object recognition. Thus far, no relation between anatomical subdivisions of TE and neuronal selectivity has been described. Here, we present evidence that neurons selective for three-dimensional (3D) shape are concentrated in the lower bank of the superior temporal sulcus, whereas neurons in lateral TE are generally unselective for 3D shape, though equally selective for 2D shape. These findings reveal that TE consists of at least two distinct areas, one of which processes a specific object property.  相似文献   

16.
This study shows that 4- and 6-month-old infants can discriminate languages (English from French) just from viewing silently presented articulations. By the age of 8 months, only bilingual (French-English) infants succeed at this task. These findings reveal a surprisingly early preparedness for visual language discrimination and highlight infants' selectivity for retaining only necessary perceptual sensitivities.  相似文献   

17.
Observers exposed alternately to a vertical grating of one spatial frequency in red light and a vertical grating of different spatial frequency in green light subsequently report frequency-specific color aftereffects when shown gratings in white light. Aftereffects occur, however, only when inspection gratings differ in spatial frequency by one octave or more and the frequency of at least one grating is above 3 cycles per degree. This spatial selectivity of the aftereffect is considered in terms of a neural adaptation model incorporating evidence on the tuning of spatial frequency detectors in the human visual system.  相似文献   

18.
王安安 《安徽农业科学》2012,40(29):14352-14354
从基于视知觉理论的色彩表现基本观点出发,通过对横道河子镇的调研,探讨了具有代表性的3个区域的色彩构成及其作用。认为区域景观色彩由主调色、界面色、点缀色3类组成,并通过它们之间的各种构成关系被观察者所认知。通过分析区域环境景观的色彩作用,根据该镇的实际情况提出了保护色彩环境的合理化建议。  相似文献   

19.
Much is known about the pathways from photoreceptors to higher visual areas in the brain. However, how we become aware of what we see or of having seen at all is a problem that has eluded neuroscience. Recordings from macaque V1 during deactivation of MT+/V5 and psychophysical studies of perceptual integration suggest that feedback from secondary visual areas to V1 is necessary for visual awareness. We used transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe the timing and function of feedback from human area MT+/V5 to V1 and found its action to be early and critical for awareness of visual motion.  相似文献   

20.
Momentary awareness of a visual scene is very limited; however, this limitation has not been formally characterized. We test the hypothesis that awareness reflects a surprisingly impoverished data structure called a labeled Boolean map, defined as a linkage of just one feature value per dimension (for example, the color is green and the motion is rightward) with a spatial pattern. Features compete with each other, whereas multiple locations form a spatial pattern and thus do not compete. Perception of the colors of two objects was significantly improved by successive compared with simultaneous presentation, whereas perception of their locations was not. Moreover, advance information about which objects are relevant aided perception of colors much more than perception of locations. Both results support the Boolean map hypothesis.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号